Review
BibTex RIS Cite

Erken Çocukluk Döneminde Bilişsel Esneklik: Esnek Amaç Odaklı Davranış Gelişimine Güncel Bakış

Year 2023, , 171 - 193, 25.08.2023
https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2022-1138580

Abstract

Bilişsel esneklik değişen hedefler ya da çevresel etkilere bağlı olarak düşünce ya da davranışlarımızı güncellememize olanak sağlayan yetenek olarak tanımlanabilir. Yirmi yılı aşkın süredir bilişsel esneklik, engelleyici kontrol ve çalışan bellek ile birlikte üç ana yürütücü işlevden biri olarak sunulmakta ve bilişsel esnekliğin çocukların belli kartları bir kuraldan (örn., şekil) diğer kurala (örn., renk) göre esnek bir şekilde sınıflandırmayı öğrendiği üç ile dört yaşları arasında geliştiği düşünülmektedir. Bilişsel esnekliğin üç ana yürütücü işlevden biri olduğunu ve üç ile dört yaşları arasında geliştiğini savunan bu klasik görüş, bilişsel esnekliğin gelişimini anlamamızda uzun yıllar etkili olmuştur. Fakat son yıllarda bilişsel esneklik konusundaki anlayışımız önemli ölçüde değişmiştir ve bu değişiklik bilişsel esnekliği daha iyi anlamamız ve daha iyi çalışmamız konusundaki değişikliklere de ön ayak olmuştur. Bu araştırma, bilişsel esnekliği çalışırken kullanılan yöntem ve analitik tekniklerdeki güncel gelişmeleri tarayarak bilişsel esneklik konusundaki güncel görüşü sunmayı amaçlamaktadır. Özellikle, son zamanlarda yapılan araştırmalar bilişsel esnekliği ölçmek için daha az sınırlandırılmış kural-değiştirme bataryaları geliştirmekte ve bilişsel esnekliği analiz etmek için daha ileri modelleme analiz yöntemlerini kullanmaktadır. Yapılan son çalışmalar, bilişsel esnekliğin okul öncesi çağda ortaya çıkan tek başına bir yürütücü işlev olmadığını göstermektedir. Aksine, son çalışmalar bilişsel esnekliğin düşünülenden daha geniş zamana yayılan ve daha farklı şekillerde ortaya çıkan kontrollü, amaç-odaklı davranış gelişiminin son basamağı olduğunu göstermektedir. Güncel veriler, uzun gelişim evresine sahip bilişsel esnekliğin yaşamın ilk iki yılındaki erken gelişim dönemlerinin dikkat ve dil yetenekleri tarafından desteklenirken, üç yaş ve sonrası ileri gelişim dönemlerinin engelleyici kontrol ve çalışan bellek yetenekleri tarafından desteklendiğini göstermektedir.

References

  • Atkinson, J., Hood, B., Wattam-Bell, J., & Braddick, O. (1992). Changes in infants’ ability to switch visual attention in the first three months of life. Perception, 21(5), 643-653. https://doi.org/10.1068/ p210643 google scholar
  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x google scholar
  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29(3), 180-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.002 google scholar
  • Blakey, E., Visser, I., & Carroll, D. J. (2016). Different executive functions support different kinds of cognitive flexibility: Evidence from 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds. Child Development, 87(2), 513-526. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12468 google scholar
  • Brooks, P. J., Hanauer, J. B., Padowska, B. & Rosman, H. (2003). The role of selective attention in preschoolers’ use in a novel dimensional card sort. Cognitive Development, 18(2), 195-215. https:// doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00020-0Buchsbaum, B. R., Greer, S., Chang, W. L., & Berman, K. F. (2005). Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin Card-Sorting task and component processes. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 35-45. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20128 google scholar
  • Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595-616. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3 google scholar
  • Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P D. (2014). Minnesota Executive Function Scale-Test Manual. St. Paul, MN: Reflection Sciences. google scholar
  • Carlson, S., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1105 -1122. https://doi. org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105 google scholar
  • Carroll, D. J., Blakey, E., & FitzGibbon, L. (2016). Cognitive flexibility in young children: Beyond perseveration. Child Development Perspectives, 10(4), 211-215. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12192 google scholar
  • Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2008). Cognitive flexibility in preschoolers: The role of representation activation and maintenance. Developmental Science, 11(3), 339-353. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1467-7687.2008.00679.x google scholar
  • Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2009). Setting goals to switch between tasks: Effect of cue transparency on children’s cognitive flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 782-797. https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0015409 google scholar
  • Chevalier, N., Sheffield, T. D., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2012). Underpinnings of the costs of flexibility in preschool children: The roles of working memory and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(2), 99-118. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.20 11.632458 google scholar
  • Clark, C. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2013). Longitudinal associations between executive control and developing mathematical competence in preschool boys and girls. Child Development, 84(2), 662-677. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01854.x google scholar
  • Clearfield, M. W., & Niman, L. C. (2012). SES affects infant cognitive flexibility. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(1), 29-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.09.007 google scholar
  • Clearfield, M. W., Diedrich, F. J., Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2006). Young infants reach correctly in A-not-B tasks: On the development of stability and perseveration. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(3), 435-444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.03.001 google scholar
  • Cragg, L., & Chevalier, N. (2012). The processes underlying flexibility in childhood. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 209-232. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903204618 google scholar
  • Cragg, L., & Nation, K. (2009). Shifting development in mid-childhood: The influence of between-task interference. Developmental Psychology, 45(5), 1465. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015360 google scholar
  • Dauvier, B., Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2012). Using a finite mixture of GLMs to explore variability in children’s flexibility in a task-switching paradigm. Cognitive Development, 27(4), 440-454. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.07.004 google scholar
  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037-2078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2006.02.006 google scholar
  • Deak, G. O., & Narasimham, G. (2003). Is perseveration caused by inhibition failure? Evidence from preschool children’s inferences about word meanings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86(3), 194-222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2003.08.001 google scholar
  • Devine, R. T., Ribner, A., & Hughes, C. (2019). Measuring and predicting individual differences in executive functions at 14 months: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 90(5), e618-e636. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13217 google scholar
  • Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB. Child Development, 56(4), 868-883. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130099 google scholar
  • Diamond, A., Carlson, S., & Beck, D. (2005). Preschool children’s performance in task switching on the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task: Separating dimensions aids the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 689-729. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_7 google scholar
  • Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 942-956. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620904771 google scholar
  • Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Bottom-up and top-down dynamics in young children’s executive function: Labels aid 3-year-olds’ performance on the Dimensional Change Card Sort. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 222-232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.12.001 google scholar
  • Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2015). A meta-analysis of the Dimensional Change Card Sort: Implications for developmental theories and the measurement of executive function in children. Developmental Review, 38, 241-268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.09.001 google scholar
  • Ellis, A. E., & Oakes, L. M. (2006). Infants flexibly use different dimensions to categorise objects. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1000. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1000 google scholar
  • Espy, K. A. (1997). The Shape School: Assessing executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(4), 495-499. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649709540690 google scholar
  • Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition:Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223-241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685 google scholar
  • Fisher, A. V. (2011a). Automatic shifts of attention in the Dimensional Change Card Sort task: Subtle changes in task materials lead to flexible switching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(1), 211-219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.001 google scholar
  • Fisher, A. V. (2011b). Processing of perceptual information is more robust than processing of conceptual information in preschool-age children: Evidence from costs of switching. Cognition, 119(2), 253264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.015 google scholar
  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31-60. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31 google scholar
  • Garon, N., Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (2014). A novel executive function battery for preschoolers: Sensitivity to age differences. Child Neuropsychology, 20(6), 713-736. https://doi.or g/10.1080/09297049.2013.857650 google scholar
  • Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [Database record]. APA PsycTests. https://doi.org/10.1037/t31298-000 google scholar
  • Hendry, A., Jones, E. J., & Charman, T. (2016). Executive function in the first three years of life: Precursors, predictors and patterns. Developmental Review, 42, 1-33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. dr.2016.06.005 google scholar
  • Holmboe, K., Fearon, R. P., Csibra, G., Tucker, L. A., & Johnson, M. H. (2008). Freeze-Frame: A new infant inhibition task and its relation to frontal cortex tasks during infancy and early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100(2), 89-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2007.09.004 google scholar
  • Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 617-644. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4 google scholar
  • Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7(3), 131-142. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-3588.00024 google scholar
  • Huizinga, M., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2007). Age-group differences in set-switching and set-maintenance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31(2), 193215. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640701190817 google scholar
  • Hunnius, S., Geuze, R. H., & Van Geert, P. (2006). Associations between the developmental trajectories of visual scanning and disengagement of attention in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(1), 108-125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.08.007 google scholar
  • Ionescu, T. (2017). The variability-stability-flexibility pattern: A possible key to understanding the flexibility of the human mind. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 123-131. https://doi. org/10.1037/gpr0000110 google scholar
  • lonescu, T. (2019). Putting the variability-stability-flexibility pattern to use: Adapting instruction to how children develop. New Ideas in Psychology, 55, 18-23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. newideapsych.2019.04.003 google scholar
  • Johansson, M., Marciszko, C., Brocki, K., & Bohlin, G. (2016). Individual differences in early executive functions: A longitudinal study from 12 to 36 months. Infant and Child Development, 25(6), 533549. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1952 google scholar
  • Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Components of visual orienting in early infancy: Contingency learning, anticipatory looking, and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(4), 335-344. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.335 google scholar
  • Jordan, P. L., & Morton, J. B. (2008). Flankers facilitate 3-year-olds’ performance in a card-sorting task. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.265 google scholar
  • Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6(5), 449-467. https://doi. org/10.1111/1467-7687.00300 google scholar
  • Kloo, D., & Perner, J. (2005). Disentangling dimensions in the dimensional change card-sorting task. Developmental Science, 8(1), 44-56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00392.x google scholar
  • Kloo, D., Perner, J., Aichhorn, M., & Schmidhuber, N. (2010). Perspective taking and cognitive flexibility in the Dimensional Change Card Sorting (DCCS) task. Cognitive Development, 25(3), 208-217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.06.001 google scholar
  • Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Vernon-Feagans, L., Blair, C. B., & Family Life Project Key Investigators (2016). The contribution of children’s time-specific and longitudinal expressive language skills on developmental trajectories of executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 148, 20-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.008 google scholar
  • Kulke, L., Atkinson, J., & Braddick, O. (2015). Automatic detection of attention shifts in infancy: Eye tracking in the fixation shift paradigm. PloS One, 10(12), e0142505. https://doi.org/10.1371/ journal.pone.0142505 google scholar
  • Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2009). A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Developmental Science, 12(1), 1-18. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00754.x google scholar
  • Marcovitch, S., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., Weaver, J. M., & Levine, D. W. (2015). A longitudinal assessment of the relation between executive function and theory of mind at 3, 4, and 5 years. Cognitive Development, 33, 40-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.07.001 google scholar
  • Miller, S. E., & Marcovitch, S. (2015). Examining executive function in the second year of life: Coherence, stability, and relations to joint attention and language. Developmental Psychology, 51 (1), 101-114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038359 google scholar
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. https://doi.org/10.1006/ cogp.1999.0734 google scholar
  • Morton, J. B., & Munakata, Y. (2002). Active versus latent representations: A neural network model of perseveration, dissociation, and decalage. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 255-265. https:// doi.org/10.1002/dev.10033 google scholar
  • Muller, U., Dick, A. S., Gela, K., Overton, W. F., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The role of negative priming in preschoolers’ flexible rule use on the Dimensional Change Card Sort task. Child Development, 77(2), 395- 412. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00878.x google scholar
  • Muller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Hood, S., Leone, T., & Rohrer, L. (2004). Interference control in a new rule use task: Age-related changes, labelling, and attention. Child Development, 75(5), 15941609. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00759.x google scholar
  • Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., & Chatham, C. H. (2012). Developing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 71-77. https://doi. org/10.1177/0963721412436807 google scholar
  • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2002). What causes 3-year-olds’ difficulty on the dimensional change card sorting task? Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 11(2), 93-105. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.299 google scholar
  • Perone, S., Palanisamy, J., & Carlson, S. M. (2018). Age-related change in brain rhythms from early to middle childhood: Links to executive function. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12691. https://doi. org/10.1111/desc.12691 google scholar
  • Phipps, D. J., Hagger, M. S., & Hamilton, K. (2019, April 24). A Meta-Analysis of Implicit and Explicit Attitudes in Children and Adolescents. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/52jrs google scholar
  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. (M. Cook, Trans.). Basic Books. google scholar
  • Reimers, S., & Maylor, E. A. (2005). Task switching across the life span: effects of age on general and specific switch costs. Developmental Psychology, 41(4), 661. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.661 google scholar
  • Rennie, D. A., Bull, R., & Diamond, A. (2004). Executive functioning in preschoolers: Reducing the inhibitory demands of the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 423-443. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2601_4 google scholar
  • Towse, J. N., Redbond, J., Houston-Price, C. M. T., & Cook, S. (2000). Understanding the dimensional change card sort: Perspectives from task success and failure. Cognitive Development, 15, 347-365. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00021-6 google scholar
  • van Bers, B. M., Visser, I., van Schijndel, T. J., Mandell, D. J., & Raijmakers, M. E. (2011). The dynamics of development on the Dimensional Change Card Sorting task. Developmental Science, 14(5), 960-971. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01045.x google scholar
  • van der Maas, H.L.J., & Straatemeier, M. (2008). How to detect cognitive strategies: Commentary on ‘Differentiation and integration: guiding principles for analysing cognitive change’. Developmental Science, 11, 449-453. https://doi.Org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00690.x google scholar
  • Wang, X., Liu, X., & Feng, T. (2021). The continuous impact of cognitive flexibility on the development of emotion understanding in children aged 4 and 5 years: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 203, 105018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105018 google scholar
  • Wiebe, S. A., & Bauer, P. J. (2005). Interference from additional props in an elicited imitation task: When in sight, firmly in mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(3), 325-363. https://doi. org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0603_2 google scholar
  • Wiebe, S. A., Lukowski, A. F., & Bauer, P. J. (2010). Sequence imitation and reaching measures of executive control: A longitudinal examination in the second year of life. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(5), 522-538. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.494751 google scholar
  • Yerys, B. E., & Munakata, Y. (2006). When labels hurt but novelty helps: Children’s perseveration and flexibility in a card-sorting task. Child Development, 77(6), 1589-1607. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1467-8624.2006.00961.x google scholar
  • Zelazo, P. D. (2004). The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 12- 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.001 google scholar
  • Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297-301. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.46 google scholar
  • Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Executive function: Reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity, and the developing brain. Developmental Review, 38, 55-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.001 google scholar
  • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11(1), 37-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(96)90027-1 google scholar

Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior

Year 2023, , 171 - 193, 25.08.2023
https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2022-1138580

Abstract

Cognitive flexibility is usually described as a skill that allows us to revise our behaviors or ideas as the task objective or the environment changes. Over 20 years, in addition to inhibitory control and working memory, cognitive flexibility has been presented as one of the main executive functions and has been considered to develop at around three to four years of age when children learn to easily switch from sorting cards according to one rule (e.g., shape) to the other rule (e.g., colour). This classic view of cognitive flexibility has been highly influential in improving our knowledge about the development of cognitive flexibility. However, in recent years, the way cognitive flexibility is understood has changed remarkably that leads the way that it should be thought and studied has also changed. The present paper aims to present a contemporary view of cognitive flexibility by reviewing recent advances in methodological and analytical techniques used to study cognitive flexibility. Particularly, recent research has started to use less constrained rule-switching tasks to assess cognitive flexibility and use more advanced modelling-based statistical approaches to analyse cognitive flexibility. These recent advances suggest that cognitive flexibility is not a stand-alone skill that emerges during the preschool years. Instead, cognitive flexibility is the culmination of the development of controlled, goal-oriented behavior that happens across a broader age span and in a much more diverse range of contexts than previously thought. While the earlier developments in cognitive flexibility in the first two years of life are underpinned by attention and language, the later developments in cognitive flexibility from around three years onwards are underpinned by inhibitory control and working memory.

References

  • Atkinson, J., Hood, B., Wattam-Bell, J., & Braddick, O. (1992). Changes in infants’ ability to switch visual attention in the first three months of life. Perception, 21(5), 643-653. https://doi.org/10.1068/ p210643 google scholar
  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x google scholar
  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29(3), 180-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.002 google scholar
  • Blakey, E., Visser, I., & Carroll, D. J. (2016). Different executive functions support different kinds of cognitive flexibility: Evidence from 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds. Child Development, 87(2), 513-526. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12468 google scholar
  • Brooks, P. J., Hanauer, J. B., Padowska, B. & Rosman, H. (2003). The role of selective attention in preschoolers’ use in a novel dimensional card sort. Cognitive Development, 18(2), 195-215. https:// doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00020-0Buchsbaum, B. R., Greer, S., Chang, W. L., & Berman, K. F. (2005). Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin Card-Sorting task and component processes. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 35-45. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20128 google scholar
  • Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595-616. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3 google scholar
  • Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P D. (2014). Minnesota Executive Function Scale-Test Manual. St. Paul, MN: Reflection Sciences. google scholar
  • Carlson, S., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1105 -1122. https://doi. org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105 google scholar
  • Carroll, D. J., Blakey, E., & FitzGibbon, L. (2016). Cognitive flexibility in young children: Beyond perseveration. Child Development Perspectives, 10(4), 211-215. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12192 google scholar
  • Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2008). Cognitive flexibility in preschoolers: The role of representation activation and maintenance. Developmental Science, 11(3), 339-353. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1467-7687.2008.00679.x google scholar
  • Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2009). Setting goals to switch between tasks: Effect of cue transparency on children’s cognitive flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 782-797. https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0015409 google scholar
  • Chevalier, N., Sheffield, T. D., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2012). Underpinnings of the costs of flexibility in preschool children: The roles of working memory and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(2), 99-118. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.20 11.632458 google scholar
  • Clark, C. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2013). Longitudinal associations between executive control and developing mathematical competence in preschool boys and girls. Child Development, 84(2), 662-677. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01854.x google scholar
  • Clearfield, M. W., & Niman, L. C. (2012). SES affects infant cognitive flexibility. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(1), 29-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.09.007 google scholar
  • Clearfield, M. W., Diedrich, F. J., Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2006). Young infants reach correctly in A-not-B tasks: On the development of stability and perseveration. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(3), 435-444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.03.001 google scholar
  • Cragg, L., & Chevalier, N. (2012). The processes underlying flexibility in childhood. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 209-232. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903204618 google scholar
  • Cragg, L., & Nation, K. (2009). Shifting development in mid-childhood: The influence of between-task interference. Developmental Psychology, 45(5), 1465. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015360 google scholar
  • Dauvier, B., Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2012). Using a finite mixture of GLMs to explore variability in children’s flexibility in a task-switching paradigm. Cognitive Development, 27(4), 440-454. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.07.004 google scholar
  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037-2078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2006.02.006 google scholar
  • Deak, G. O., & Narasimham, G. (2003). Is perseveration caused by inhibition failure? Evidence from preschool children’s inferences about word meanings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86(3), 194-222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2003.08.001 google scholar
  • Devine, R. T., Ribner, A., & Hughes, C. (2019). Measuring and predicting individual differences in executive functions at 14 months: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 90(5), e618-e636. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13217 google scholar
  • Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB. Child Development, 56(4), 868-883. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130099 google scholar
  • Diamond, A., Carlson, S., & Beck, D. (2005). Preschool children’s performance in task switching on the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task: Separating dimensions aids the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 689-729. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_7 google scholar
  • Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 942-956. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620904771 google scholar
  • Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Bottom-up and top-down dynamics in young children’s executive function: Labels aid 3-year-olds’ performance on the Dimensional Change Card Sort. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 222-232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.12.001 google scholar
  • Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2015). A meta-analysis of the Dimensional Change Card Sort: Implications for developmental theories and the measurement of executive function in children. Developmental Review, 38, 241-268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.09.001 google scholar
  • Ellis, A. E., & Oakes, L. M. (2006). Infants flexibly use different dimensions to categorise objects. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1000. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1000 google scholar
  • Espy, K. A. (1997). The Shape School: Assessing executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(4), 495-499. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649709540690 google scholar
  • Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition:Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223-241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685 google scholar
  • Fisher, A. V. (2011a). Automatic shifts of attention in the Dimensional Change Card Sort task: Subtle changes in task materials lead to flexible switching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(1), 211-219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.001 google scholar
  • Fisher, A. V. (2011b). Processing of perceptual information is more robust than processing of conceptual information in preschool-age children: Evidence from costs of switching. Cognition, 119(2), 253264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.015 google scholar
  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31-60. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31 google scholar
  • Garon, N., Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (2014). A novel executive function battery for preschoolers: Sensitivity to age differences. Child Neuropsychology, 20(6), 713-736. https://doi.or g/10.1080/09297049.2013.857650 google scholar
  • Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [Database record]. APA PsycTests. https://doi.org/10.1037/t31298-000 google scholar
  • Hendry, A., Jones, E. J., & Charman, T. (2016). Executive function in the first three years of life: Precursors, predictors and patterns. Developmental Review, 42, 1-33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. dr.2016.06.005 google scholar
  • Holmboe, K., Fearon, R. P., Csibra, G., Tucker, L. A., & Johnson, M. H. (2008). Freeze-Frame: A new infant inhibition task and its relation to frontal cortex tasks during infancy and early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100(2), 89-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2007.09.004 google scholar
  • Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 617-644. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4 google scholar
  • Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7(3), 131-142. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-3588.00024 google scholar
  • Huizinga, M., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2007). Age-group differences in set-switching and set-maintenance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31(2), 193215. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640701190817 google scholar
  • Hunnius, S., Geuze, R. H., & Van Geert, P. (2006). Associations between the developmental trajectories of visual scanning and disengagement of attention in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(1), 108-125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.08.007 google scholar
  • Ionescu, T. (2017). The variability-stability-flexibility pattern: A possible key to understanding the flexibility of the human mind. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 123-131. https://doi. org/10.1037/gpr0000110 google scholar
  • lonescu, T. (2019). Putting the variability-stability-flexibility pattern to use: Adapting instruction to how children develop. New Ideas in Psychology, 55, 18-23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. newideapsych.2019.04.003 google scholar
  • Johansson, M., Marciszko, C., Brocki, K., & Bohlin, G. (2016). Individual differences in early executive functions: A longitudinal study from 12 to 36 months. Infant and Child Development, 25(6), 533549. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1952 google scholar
  • Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Components of visual orienting in early infancy: Contingency learning, anticipatory looking, and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(4), 335-344. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.335 google scholar
  • Jordan, P. L., & Morton, J. B. (2008). Flankers facilitate 3-year-olds’ performance in a card-sorting task. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.265 google scholar
  • Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6(5), 449-467. https://doi. org/10.1111/1467-7687.00300 google scholar
  • Kloo, D., & Perner, J. (2005). Disentangling dimensions in the dimensional change card-sorting task. Developmental Science, 8(1), 44-56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00392.x google scholar
  • Kloo, D., Perner, J., Aichhorn, M., & Schmidhuber, N. (2010). Perspective taking and cognitive flexibility in the Dimensional Change Card Sorting (DCCS) task. Cognitive Development, 25(3), 208-217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.06.001 google scholar
  • Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Vernon-Feagans, L., Blair, C. B., & Family Life Project Key Investigators (2016). The contribution of children’s time-specific and longitudinal expressive language skills on developmental trajectories of executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 148, 20-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.008 google scholar
  • Kulke, L., Atkinson, J., & Braddick, O. (2015). Automatic detection of attention shifts in infancy: Eye tracking in the fixation shift paradigm. PloS One, 10(12), e0142505. https://doi.org/10.1371/ journal.pone.0142505 google scholar
  • Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2009). A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Developmental Science, 12(1), 1-18. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00754.x google scholar
  • Marcovitch, S., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., Weaver, J. M., & Levine, D. W. (2015). A longitudinal assessment of the relation between executive function and theory of mind at 3, 4, and 5 years. Cognitive Development, 33, 40-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.07.001 google scholar
  • Miller, S. E., & Marcovitch, S. (2015). Examining executive function in the second year of life: Coherence, stability, and relations to joint attention and language. Developmental Psychology, 51 (1), 101-114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038359 google scholar
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. https://doi.org/10.1006/ cogp.1999.0734 google scholar
  • Morton, J. B., & Munakata, Y. (2002). Active versus latent representations: A neural network model of perseveration, dissociation, and decalage. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 255-265. https:// doi.org/10.1002/dev.10033 google scholar
  • Muller, U., Dick, A. S., Gela, K., Overton, W. F., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The role of negative priming in preschoolers’ flexible rule use on the Dimensional Change Card Sort task. Child Development, 77(2), 395- 412. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00878.x google scholar
  • Muller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Hood, S., Leone, T., & Rohrer, L. (2004). Interference control in a new rule use task: Age-related changes, labelling, and attention. Child Development, 75(5), 15941609. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00759.x google scholar
  • Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., & Chatham, C. H. (2012). Developing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 71-77. https://doi. org/10.1177/0963721412436807 google scholar
  • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2002). What causes 3-year-olds’ difficulty on the dimensional change card sorting task? Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 11(2), 93-105. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.299 google scholar
  • Perone, S., Palanisamy, J., & Carlson, S. M. (2018). Age-related change in brain rhythms from early to middle childhood: Links to executive function. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12691. https://doi. org/10.1111/desc.12691 google scholar
  • Phipps, D. J., Hagger, M. S., & Hamilton, K. (2019, April 24). A Meta-Analysis of Implicit and Explicit Attitudes in Children and Adolescents. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/52jrs google scholar
  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. (M. Cook, Trans.). Basic Books. google scholar
  • Reimers, S., & Maylor, E. A. (2005). Task switching across the life span: effects of age on general and specific switch costs. Developmental Psychology, 41(4), 661. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.661 google scholar
  • Rennie, D. A., Bull, R., & Diamond, A. (2004). Executive functioning in preschoolers: Reducing the inhibitory demands of the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 423-443. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2601_4 google scholar
  • Towse, J. N., Redbond, J., Houston-Price, C. M. T., & Cook, S. (2000). Understanding the dimensional change card sort: Perspectives from task success and failure. Cognitive Development, 15, 347-365. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00021-6 google scholar
  • van Bers, B. M., Visser, I., van Schijndel, T. J., Mandell, D. J., & Raijmakers, M. E. (2011). The dynamics of development on the Dimensional Change Card Sorting task. Developmental Science, 14(5), 960-971. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01045.x google scholar
  • van der Maas, H.L.J., & Straatemeier, M. (2008). How to detect cognitive strategies: Commentary on ‘Differentiation and integration: guiding principles for analysing cognitive change’. Developmental Science, 11, 449-453. https://doi.Org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00690.x google scholar
  • Wang, X., Liu, X., & Feng, T. (2021). The continuous impact of cognitive flexibility on the development of emotion understanding in children aged 4 and 5 years: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 203, 105018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105018 google scholar
  • Wiebe, S. A., & Bauer, P. J. (2005). Interference from additional props in an elicited imitation task: When in sight, firmly in mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(3), 325-363. https://doi. org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0603_2 google scholar
  • Wiebe, S. A., Lukowski, A. F., & Bauer, P. J. (2010). Sequence imitation and reaching measures of executive control: A longitudinal examination in the second year of life. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(5), 522-538. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.494751 google scholar
  • Yerys, B. E., & Munakata, Y. (2006). When labels hurt but novelty helps: Children’s perseveration and flexibility in a card-sorting task. Child Development, 77(6), 1589-1607. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1467-8624.2006.00961.x google scholar
  • Zelazo, P. D. (2004). The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 12- 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.001 google scholar
  • Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297-301. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.46 google scholar
  • Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Executive function: Reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity, and the developing brain. Developmental Review, 38, 55-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.001 google scholar
  • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11(1), 37-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(96)90027-1 google scholar
There are 75 citations in total.

Details

Primary Language English
Subjects Social and Personality Psychology (Other)
Journal Section Review
Authors

Yesim Yavaslar Dogru 0000-0002-8229-0528

Daniel Carroll 0000-0003-2190-7253

Emma Blakey 0000-0003-3685-3649

Publication Date August 25, 2023
Submission Date June 30, 2022
Published in Issue Year 2023

Cite

APA Yavaslar Dogru, Y., Carroll, D., & Blakey, E. (2023). Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior. Studies in Psychology, 43(2), 171-193. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2022-1138580
AMA Yavaslar Dogru Y, Carroll D, Blakey E. Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior. Studies in Psychology. August 2023;43(2):171-193. doi:10.26650/SP2022-1138580
Chicago Yavaslar Dogru, Yesim, Daniel Carroll, and Emma Blakey. “Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior”. Studies in Psychology 43, no. 2 (August 2023): 171-93. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2022-1138580.
EndNote Yavaslar Dogru Y, Carroll D, Blakey E (August 1, 2023) Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior. Studies in Psychology 43 2 171–193.
IEEE Y. Yavaslar Dogru, D. Carroll, and E. Blakey, “Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior”, Studies in Psychology, vol. 43, no. 2, pp. 171–193, 2023, doi: 10.26650/SP2022-1138580.
ISNAD Yavaslar Dogru, Yesim et al. “Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior”. Studies in Psychology 43/2 (August 2023), 171-193. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2022-1138580.
JAMA Yavaslar Dogru Y, Carroll D, Blakey E. Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior. Studies in Psychology. 2023;43:171–193.
MLA Yavaslar Dogru, Yesim et al. “Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior”. Studies in Psychology, vol. 43, no. 2, 2023, pp. 171-93, doi:10.26650/SP2022-1138580.
Vancouver Yavaslar Dogru Y, Carroll D, Blakey E. Cognitive Flexibility in Early Childhood: A Contemporary View of the Development of Flexible Goal-Oriented Behavior. Studies in Psychology. 2023;43(2):171-93.

Psikoloji Çalışmaları / Studies In Psychology / ISSN- 1304-4680